Smart Drugs & LTP: Enhancement of Long-term Potentiation Through Actions on AMPA-Receptor Initiation and CREB Consolidation
Unlike the previous installment, this post presumes a great deal of background knowledge on the part of its readers. My intended audience has taken a class in general biology, neuroscience, or physiology at the college level and are familiar with terms like synapse, action potential (depolarization, ion channel, electrochemical gradient), neurotransmission (axons, dendrites, receptors, ligands, inhibition (IPSP), excitation (EPSP)), neurotransmitters (especially glutamate, GABA, acetylcholine, dopamine, serotonin, epinephrine), and gene transcription/translation. Anyone who's not conversant in biochemistry may want to take a minute and bone up on this stuff, perhaps by reading my first post or by just skimming the links above.
Drug-mediated
cognitive enhancement has become a topic of great interest among
researchers and laypeople alike, but still precious little is known
about the neurobiology underlying our cognitive processes. Over the past two million years—a paltry interval in evolutionary time—growth of the human brain has wildly
outpaced that of our closest relatives. But it is also true that these expanded cortical areas are undergirded by neural circuitry that we
share with our primate, and indeed our reptilian, ancestors. Since complex phenotypes neverarise de novo, it is unlikely that human “brain plans” are in any
way optimized for cognition; rather, this growth seems to be in line
with an inherited simian blueprint1.
All this is to suggest that presently, our brains fall far short of
maximizing these recent specializations that confer human uniqueness.
If this assumption is a safe one, then it follows that the current
state of our cognition leaves much room for improvement.
The
foregoing discussion presents an unsettling scenario fraught with both scientific and ethical dilemmata. Still, our
understanding of learning and memory can be greatly enriched by a
consideration of these performance enhancing drugs and their effects, both in the
medically compromised and in the neurotypical. However, this post does not address the wider world of nootropic substances; my focus is limited to the effects compounds known to faciliate LTP. As long-term potentiation (LTP)
is considered to be the major cellular mechanism underlying learning
and memory, I seek to examine how this process can be exogenously enhanced by pharmacological tinkering at the two
crucial phases of the process: the initiation of memory formation
during early long-term potentiation and memory consolidation during
late long-term potentiation.
LTP OVERVIEW: SKIP IF YOU GET IT!
Before
considering the affects of a given substance on long-term
potentiation, it would be prudent to give both an overview of the
neuromolecular correlates of these phenomena and due homage to those
who discovered them. Donald O. Hebb was a Canadian psychologist whose
pioneering efforts in the field earned him the moniker “father of
neural networks2.” He postulated
that the efficacy of synaptic communication was dependent on activity
at the synapse; that strong, repeated activation of these connections
can result in lasting structural and functional changes. In his
words, “when an axon of cell A is near enough to excite cell
B and repeatedly or persistently takes part in firing it, some growth
process or metabolic change takes place in one or both cells such
that A's efficiency, as one of the cells firing B, is increased2.”
His idea that modifications in neural circuits were the
mechanisms by which information is stored and retrieved in the brain
was borne out in the research of Terje Lømo, who in 1966 discovered
in the rabbit hippocampus what would come to be known as long-term
potentiation3. Studies exploring
the nature of this effect, along with work done by Eric Kandel on
habituation and sensitization of neural circuits in Aplysia californica 4, served as a
springboard for recent discoveries in the molecular basis of memory
and gave the model of synaptic plasticity its modern form.
Aplysia californica, by Nordelch |
Long-term potentiation is the name given to the discovery that a
brief trains of high-frequency stimuli to monosynaptic excitatory
pathways in the hippocampus cause a sustained increase in the
efficiency of synaptic transmission5.
This effect persists for hours in ex vivo hippocampal slices and for
weeks in the hippocampus of living mammals. LTP is commonly divided
into two phases—early LTP and late LTP—each consisting of three
processes: induction, maintenance, and expression6.
In such a monosynaptic excitatory pathway, a stimulus causes
presynaptic release of glutamate onto the postsynaptic cell membrane
where it binds to α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionicacid (AMPA) receptors. This triggers the influx of sodium
cations into the postsynaptic cell, effecting an excitatory
postsynaptic potential through depolarization. High frequency
stimulation of this sort results in EPSP summation and greater
depolarization of the postsynaptic cell. If sufficient, this
depolarization causes the ejection of Mg2+ from the ion
channel of N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors, effectively
unblocking them. Now, if these receptors are also bound by glutamate released from the
presynaptic cell, they become active and allow Ca2+ to
flow into the postsynaptic cell. This rapid rise in intracellular
calcium concentration is the pivotal step for subsequent signaling
cascades; it activates several enzymes that mediate early LTP
induction such as Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinases II(CaMKII), protein kinase C (PKC), and to a lesser extent proteinkinase A (PKA) and Mitogen-activated protein kinase K (MAPK)6. All kinases are enzymes that affect the state of a molecule (changing its activity, reactivity, or binding ability) by simply sticking a phosphate group on it; they serve many important functions in the regulation of complex cellular processes. Through persistent
activation of these kinases (particularly CaMKII and PKC), existing AMPA receptors
are phosphorylated (activated) and additional AMPA receptors are
inserted into the postsynaptic membrane, both of which increase postsynaptic response to
released glutamate so future excitatory stimuli generate larger
postsynaptic potentials. In addition, CaMKII may lead to the
synthesis of a retrograde messenger that acts to increase presynaptic
neurotransmitter vesicle number, probability of vesicle release, or
both6. The process described above
accounts for the few hours of LTP observed in ex vivo hippocampal
tissue. Functional modification of the circuit must rely on changes
in protein synthesis or alterations in the rate of synthesis and
degradation of proteins already present.
The
late phase of LTP is induced by changes gene transcription and
protein synthesis brought about by the persistent activation of
protein kinases activated during early LTP, such as MAPK1;
6. This process is necessary for memory
formation; it has been show in many studies that inhibition of protein synthesis disrupts late LTP7.
PKA activation and calcium influx converge on CRTC1, a potent
transcription factor for cAMP response element binding protein(CREB). Through phosphorylation-dependent activation this molecule affects the transcription of many genes,
including genes encoding other transcription factors8
and genes involved in synaptic plasticity9.
CREB-mediated transcriptional activity is important in habituation
and sensitization as well10 and
is a good candidate for mediating the molecular switch to long-term
memory. Research continues to suggest that CREB plays an important role in memory formation and retrieval.
This brief description of LTP belies its complexity and diversity; in truth, much about it remains to be discovered and the list of potential modulators (molecules that can alter LTP but are not essential for it) is ever-growing. For instance, beta-adrenergicreceptor agonists, nitric oxide synthase, and estradiol have all been proposed to have an effect on LTP6. What follows will be limited to a consideration of the known effects of certain drugs on AMPA receptors and CREB-mediated transcriptional activity in NMDA receptor-dependent hippocampal LTP.
This brief description of LTP belies its complexity and diversity; in truth, much about it remains to be discovered and the list of potential modulators (molecules that can alter LTP but are not essential for it) is ever-growing. For instance, beta-adrenergicreceptor agonists, nitric oxide synthase, and estradiol have all been proposed to have an effect on LTP6. What follows will be limited to a consideration of the known effects of certain drugs on AMPA receptors and CREB-mediated transcriptional activity in NMDA receptor-dependent hippocampal LTP.
SUBSTANCES THAT AFFECT LTP
To
achieve an enhancement of memory through a direct effect on LTP, a
drug can act at either the early phase or the late phase of the
process described above. In the case of the early phase, the
ionotropic glutamatergic receptors are obvious targets for these
drugs. A class of pyrrolidine-derived compounds known as racetams
bind to modulator sites on the AMPA receptor, including the
cyclothiazide site, and have been found to have a positive effect on
memory11. Piracetam is the most
well-known racetam and was the first of this class of molecules to be
discovered12. In cultured
neurons, it enhances the Ca2+ influx
produced by the AMPA receptor but not that produced by the NMDA
receptor13. In
electrophysiological studies it increases the peak amplitude of the
ion current generated through AMPA receptors, reduces it rate of
decay, and increases the maximal density of low-affinity binding
sites for AMPA in the postsynaptic membrane13.
It also increases muscarinic cholinergic receptor density in the
frontal cortex of mice and has the general effect of activating the
cholinergic system14. Given the
dual action of the these compounds, choline and piracetam administered
together have been shown to substantially improve memory in dementia
patients15. Indeed, a reduction in the activity of cholinergic neurons is a well-known feature of Alzheimer's disease, and several modern treatments for mild to moderate Alzheimer's are based on increasing the concentration of acetylcholine in
the brain. Piracetam alone has
been shown to effectively overcome amnesia induced by scopolamine,
diazepam, and electroconvulsive shock through actions on the
hippocampus16. Aniracetam, a
second member of the racetam family, has been known to improve
cognitive functions impaired in rodents by experimental procedures
since the early 1980s17. It
facilitates LTP in the same way as piracetam, slowing entry of AMPA
receptors into a desensitized state and increasing excitatory
synaptic strength18, but it also
seems to enhance cortical GABA-mediated inhibition19.
Aniracetam facilitates LTP in hippocampal tissue20,
has improved performance of rhesus monkeys in delayed match-to-sampletasks21, and reverses learning
impairment in rodents22.
Ampakines, close relatives of the racetam family, were the first allosteric modulators of AMPA receptors found to be able to augment excitatory transmission in the brain20; 23. They consist of thiazide derivatives and unlike their parent compound they are able to cross the blood-brain barrier to bind to the cyclothiazide binding site of the AMPA receptor, slowing receptor desensitization and deactivation. Since racetams and ampakines are allosteric modulators, they affect only AMPA receptors activated by endogenous transmitter and thereby restrict their influence to regions that are engaged in brain activity24. Ampakines have also been found to induce the expression of neurotrophin genes, such as growth factors like BDNF20. They have also been found to improve delayed recall in aged individuals and to facilitate memory encoding generally25; 26. In all, racetams and ampakines act similarly to slow deactivation and attenuate desensitization of AMPA receptor currents, increase synaptic responses, and enhance long-term potentiation.
In
the late phase of LTP, the transcription factor CREB has been shown
to be crucial to memory consolidation—its loss of function results
in an impairment of long-term memory, while increases in CREB
activity enhance long-term memory; importantly CREB activity does not seem to
affect short-term memory27.
CREB-dependent gene expression is mainly regulated through
phosphorylation and through chromatin remodeling27;
28. Inhibition of phosphodiesterase (PDE) activity leads to increases in cAMP or cGMP levels, which drives CREB
phosphorylation and activation
through increases in protein kinase activity . Whether cAMP or cGMP is increased
depends on the phosphodiesterase—PDE4 inhibition increases cAMP
levels while PDE5 inhibition increases cGMP levels29.
The prototypical PDE4 inhibitor is rolipram, which leads to CREB
phosphorylation and CREB-dependent gene transcription though
activation of PKA30. In animal
models, rolipram has been shown to facilitate memory formation by
increasing CREB phosphorylation.29
Epigenetic
chromatin remodeling and modifications of DNA represent central
mechanisms for regulation of gene expression during memory formation.
In order for gene expression to take place, chromatin must be
unpacked to expose DNA regulatory sequences to transcription factors
such as CREB31. A primary
mechanism for attaining the chromatin state required for
transcriptional activity is histone acetylation, which depends on the
relative activities of enzymes histone-acetyl transferase (HAT) and
histone deacetylase (HDAC). To promote long-term memory related gene
expression, CREB requires a coactivator called CREB binding protein (CBP). This coactivator possesses histone acetyl-transferase activity
required for transcription32. CBP
histone acetyl-transferase activity is an important component in
memory consolidation; truncated
CBP protein in transgenic mice significantly reduced late-phase LTP in hippocampal
slices. These mice also exhibited behavioral deficits in two
hippocampus-dependent tasks: spatial learning in the Morris watermaze and long-term memory for contextual fear conditioning.
Corroborating these findings, it has been shown that elevated levels
of histone acetylation through the use of HDAC inhibitors such as
sodium butyrate enhances induction of long-term potentiation at
Schaffer-collateral synapses in the hippocampus in vitro as well as
long-term memory formation in a contextual fear conditioning
paradigm33.
An
examination of two well-characterized steps in the process of
long-term potentiation has recommended three key processes through
which its enhancement could be mediated: AMPA receptor modulation,
phosphodiesterase inhibition, and histone deacetylase inhibition. The
compounds promoting these processes have the potential to be
therapeutically valuable in cases of cognitive impairment. This is
illustrative of just how labile the processes underlying our
behavioral memory actually are.
1. Finlay, B. L., Darlington, R. B. & Nicastro, N.
(2001). Developmental structure in brain evolution. Behavioral
and Brain Sciences 24,
263-+.
2. Brown, R. E. & Milner, P. M. (2003). Timeline -
The legacy of Donald O. Hebb: more than the Hebb synapse. Nature
Reviews Neuroscience 4,
1013-1019.
3. Lomo, T. (2003). The discovery of long-term
potentiation. Philosophical Transactions of
the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences
358, 617-620.
4. Pittenger, C. & Kandel, E. R. (2003). In search
of general mechanisms for long-lasting plasticity: Aplysia and the
hippocampus. Philosophical Transactions of the
Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences
358, 757-763.
5. Bliss, T. V. P. & Collingridge, G. L. (1993). A
SYNAPTIC MODEL OF MEMORY - LONG-TERM POTENTIATION IN THE HIPPOCAMPUS.
Nature 361,
31-39.
6. Sweatt, J. D. (1999). Toward a molecular explanation
for long-term potentiation. Learning &
Memory 6,
399-416.
7. Frey, U. & Morris, R. G. M. (1997). Synaptic
tagging and long-term potentiation. Nature
385, 533-536.
8. Shaywitz, A. J. & Greenberg, M. E. (1999). CREB:
A stimulus-induced transcription factor activated by a diverse array
of extracellular signals. Annual Review of
Biochemistry 68,
821-861.
9. Pfenning, A. R., Schwartz, R. & Barth, A. L.
(2007). A comparative genomics approach to identifying the plasticity
transcriptome. Bmc Neuroscience
8, 18.
10. Waddell, S. & Quinn, W. G. (2001). Flies, genes,
and learning. Annual Review of Neuroscience
24, 1283-1309.
11. Sara, S. J., Davidremacle, M., Weyers, M. &
Giurgea, C. (1979). PIRACETAM FACILITATES RETRIEVAL BUT DOES NOT
IMPAIR EXTINCTION OF BAR-PRESSING IN RATS. Psychopharmacology
61, 71-75.
12. Giurgea, C. (1972). Pharmacology of integrative
activity of the brain. Attempt at nootropic concept in
psychopharmacology. Actualites
pharmacologiques 25,
115-56.
13. Nicoletti, F., Casabona, G., Genazzani, A. A.,
Copani, A., Aleppo, G., Canonico, P. L. & Scapagnini, U. (1992).
Excitatory amino acids and neuronal plasticity: modulation of AMPA
receptors as a novel substrate for the action of nootropic drugs.
Functional neurology
7, 413-22.
14. Pepeu, G. & Spignoli, G. (1989). NOOTROPIC DRUGS
AND BRAIN CHOLINERGIC MECHANISMS. Progress in
Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry
13, S77-S88.
15. Bartus, R. T., Dean, R. L., Sherman, K. A.,
Friedman, E. & Beer, B. (1981). PROFOUND EFFECTS OF COMBINING
CHOLINE AND PIRACETAM ON MEMORY ENHANCEMENT AND CHOLINERGIC FUNCTION
IN AGED RATS. Neurobiology of Aging
2, 105-111.
16. Lenegre, A., Chermat, R., Avril, I., Steru, L. &
Porsolt, R. D. (1988). SPECIFICITY OF PIRACETAM ANTI-AMNESIC ACTIVITY
IN 3 MODELS OF AMNESIA IN T HE MOUSE. Pharmacology
Biochemistry and Behavior 29,
625-629.
17. Cumin, R., Bandle, E. F., Gamzu, E. & Haefely,
W. E. (1982). EFFECTS OF THE NOVEL COMPOUND ANIRACETAM (RO 13-5057)
UPON IMPAIRED LEARNING AND MEMORY IN RODENTS. Psychopharmacology
78, 104-111.
18. Vyklicky, L., Patneau, D. K. & Mayer, M. L.
(1991). MODULATION OF EXCITATORY SYNAPTIC TRANSMISSION BY DRUGS THAT
REDUCE DESENSITIZATION AT AMPA KAINATE RECEPTORS. Neuron
7, 971-984.
19. Ling, D. S. F. & Benardo, L. S. (2005).
Nootropic agents enhance the recruitment of fast GABA(A) inhibition
in rat neocortex. Cerebral Cortex
15, 921-928.
20. Arai, A. C. & Kessler, M. (2007). Pharmacology
of ampakine modulators: From AMPA receptors to synapses and behavior.
Current Drug Targets
8, 583-602.
21. Buccafusco, J. J., Weiser, T., Winter, K., Klinder,
K. & Terry, A. V. (2004). The effects of IDRA 21, a positive
modulator of the AMPA receptor, on delayed matching performance by
young and aged rhesus monkeys. Neuropharmacology
46, 10-22.
22. Martin, J. R., Moreau, J. L. & Jenck, F. (1995).
ANIRACETAM REVERSES MEMORY IMPAIRMENT IN RATS. Pharmacological
Research 31,
133-136.
23. Staubli, U., Ambrosingerson, J. & Lynch, G.
(1992). RECEPTOR CHANGES AND LTP - AN ANALYSIS USING ANIRACETAM, A
DRUG THAT REVERSIBLY MODIFIES GLUTAMATE (AMPA) RECEPTORS. Hippocampus
2, 49-58.
24. O'Neill, M. J. & Witkin, J. M. (2007). AMPA
receptor potentiators: Application for depression and Parkinson's
disease. Current Drug Targets
8, 603-620.
25. Davis, C. M., Moskovitz, B., Nguyen, M. A., Tran, B.
B., Arai, A., Lynch, G. & Granger, R. (1997). A profile of the
behavioral changes produced by facilitation of AMPA-type glutamate
receptors. Psychopharmacology
133, 161-167.
26. Ingvar, M., AmbrosIngerson, J., Davis, M., Granger,
R., Kessler, M., Rogers, G. A., Schehr, R. S. & Lynch, G. (1997).
Enhancement by an ampakine of memory encoding in humans. Experimental
Neurology 146,
553-559.
27. Tully, T., Bourtchouladze, R., Scott, R. &
Tallman, J. (2003). Targeting the CREB pathway for memory enhancers.
Nature Reviews Drug Discovery
2, 267-277.
28. Rose, G. M., Hopper, A., De Vivo, M. & Tehim, A.
(2005). Phosphodiesterase inhibitors for cognitive enhancement.
Current Pharmaceutical Design
11, 3329-3334.
29. Blokland, A., Schreiber, R. & Prickaerts, J.
(2006). Improving memory: A role for phosphodiesterases. Current
Pharmaceutical Design 12,
2511-2523.
30. Arnsten, A. F. T., Ramos, B. P., Birnbaum, S. G. &
Taylor, J. R. (2005). Protein kinase A as a therapeutic target for
memory disorders: rationale and challenges. Trends
in Molecular Medicine 11,
121-128.
31. Santos, M., Coelho, P. A. & Maciel, P. (2006).
Chromatin remodeling and neuronal function: exciting links. Genes
Brain and Behavior 5,
80-91.
32. Bannister, A. J. & Kouzarides, T. (1996). The
CBP co-activator is a histone acetyltransferase. Nature
384, 641-643.
33. Levenson, J. M. & Sweatt, J. D. (2006).
Epigenetic mechanisms: a common theme in vertebrate and invertebrate
memory formation. Cellular and Molecular Life
Sciences 63,
1009-1016.
No comments:
Post a Comment